American Journal of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine

Intermittent hypoxia (IH), one of the hallmarks of obstructive sleep apnea, occurs more frequently during pregnancy. We hypothesized that IH may lead to persistent postnatal changes in respiratory responses to acute hypoxia and may also lead to adverse effects on spatial function learning as revealed by the Morris water maze. To examine this issue, time-pregnant Sprague-Dawley rats were exposed to IH and room air (IHRA; 21 and 10% O2 alternations every 90 seconds) or to normoxia (RARA) until delivery. Ventilatory and metabolic responses to a 20-minute acute hypoxic challenge (10% O2) were conducted at postnatal ages 5, 10, 15, and 30 days. In addition, spatial task learning was assessed in the water maze at 1 and 4 months of age. Normoxic ventilation was higher at all time points in IHRA rats than in RARA rats (p < 0.01). Peak hypoxic ventilatory responses were attenuated in IHRA rats at 5 days of age and hypoxic ventilatory depression was accentuated at this age as well. However, ventilatory equivalents (minute ventilation/oxygen consumption) revealed significant reductions in peak hypoxic ventilatory responses of IHRA rats and hypoxic ventilatory depression at all postnatal ages (p < 0.01). Acquisition and retention of a spatial task were similar in the IHRA and RARA groups at both 1 and 4 months of age. We conclude that gestational intermittent hypoxia elicits long-lasting alterations in the control of breathing. We postulate that such IH-induced respiratory plasticity may create selective vulnerability to hypoxia during development.

Environmental conditions during fetal and early postnatal life can induce profound alterations in the developmental characteristics of respiratory behaviors as well as other complex behaviors such as learned behaviors and spatial memory tasks (16). It has become clear that perturbations in gas exchange during early postnatal life can lead to marked modifications in the respiratory phenotype. For example, exposure to hyperoxic conditions during the first month of life will be associated with lifelong attenuations of peripheral chemoreceptor function (7). Similarly, in both rats and sheep, sustained postnatal hypoxemia has been shown to attenuate the hypoxic ventilatory response, an effect that may last until adulthood (8, 9). Thus, the brain regions underlying respiratory control mechanisms are susceptible to long-lasting forms of plasticity.

Intermittent rather than sustained long-lasting hypoxia is more likely to occur not only postnatally but also during fetal life. A typical example is the occurrence of obstructive sleep apnea in pregnant women. Indeed, the second half of human pregnancy may carry a substantially higher risk for developing this condition, which is characteristically associated with recurring cycles of hypoxemia during sleep (10). Although such events appear to affect fetal somatic growth as evidenced by lower birthweights among snoring women (11), the overall impact of such gas disturbances on postnatal respiratory control remains unknown. Peyronnet and colleagues have shown that sustained prenatal hypoxia leads to substantial alterations in normoxic ventilation for a period of at least 3 weeks postnatally (2). In addition, we have shown that intermittent hypoxia (IH) leads to substantial disruption of the ability to acquire a spatial memory task in adult rats (12), and that the hippocampal vulnerability to IH is particularly prominent during early postnatal life (13). However, the potential effect of prenatal IH on hippocampal function was not examined.

On the basis of the aforementioned considerations, we hypothesized that prenatal intermittent hypoxia would lead to sustained modifications in ventilatory control and would also adversely impact on the ability to acquire a learned spatial task in the Morris water maze.

Time-pregnant Sprague-Dawley rats were purchased from Charles River Laboratories (Wilmington, MA) and used for all experiments. The experimental protocols were approved by the Institutional Animal Use and Care Committee and are in close agreement with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (14). All efforts were made to minimize animal suffering and to reduce the number of animals used.

Intermittent Hypoxia Protocol

Animals were placed from Day 5 of pregnancy until delivery in computer-controlled environmental chambers, in which the moment-to-moment desired oxygen concentration of the chamber was programmed and adjusted automatically. The IH profile consisted of alternating room air (RA) and 10% oxygen every 90 seconds from Day 5 of pregnancy until delivery during the light phase. Matched control animals were exposed to circulating normoxic gas in one of the chambers. Within less than 6 hours after delivery, litters were removed from the chambers, culled to eight pups, and maintained in normoxic conditions until measurements (see online supplement).

Ventilatory and Cardiovascular Recordings

Cardiorespiratory measures were continuously acquired in the freely behaving animals, using the barometric method (Buxco Electronics, Troy, NY) (15, 16) with appropriate corrections for inspiratory and expiratory barometric asymmetries (17). Environmental temperature was maintained slightly below the thermoneutral range (26–31°C dependent on postnatal age as previously described [18]). Animals were allowed to acclimate to the chamber, and pressure changes due to the inspiratory and expiratory temperature changes were measured with a high-gain differential pressure transducer (model MP45-1; Validyne, Northridge, CA) (19). Analog signals were continuously digitized, and analyzed on-line by a microcomputer software program (Buxco Electronics). A rejection algorithm was included in the breath-by-breath analysis routine and allowed for accurate rejection of motion-induced artifacts. Tidal volume (Vt), respiratory frequency (f), minute ventilation (V̇e), mean inspiratory flow (Vt/Ti), and inspiratory duty cycle (Ti/Ttot) were computed and stored for subsequent off-line analysis (see online supplement).

Acute Hypoxic Ventilatory Challenges

Rat pups were studied on postnatal Days 5, 10, 15, and 30. The animals were weighed and placed in the barometric recording chamber. After stable baseline normoxic values were obtained for at least 5 minutes, rats were switched to 10% O2 (balance N2) using a premixed gas mixture. The hypoxic challenge lasted for 20 minutes, after which room air was reintroduced in the recording chamber, and recovery was recorded for 10 minutes.

In an additional eight IHRA and eight normoxia (RARA) 5-day-old pups, the Dejours test was conducted to assess the overall sensitivity of peripheral chemoreceptors. To this effect, after a stable normoxic baseline was established, 100% O2 was flushed into the whole body plethysmograph, and the overall change in ventilation over the initial 15 seconds from gas transition was determined as peripheral chemoreceptor gain (20).

Ventilatory measures were averaged at 1-minute intervals and plotted. Analyses of variance (ANOVAs; one-way or two-way) were employed to compare normoxic ventilation, peak ventilatory response to hypoxia (pHVR), and hypoxic ventilatory depression (HVD). Significant comparisons were followed by Newman–Keuls tests. A p value < 0.05 was considered to achieve statistical significance.

Metabolic Rate

Metabolic rate was measured with a computer-controlled indirect calorimetry system similar to that described by Jensen and colleagues (21). Air taken from a common air source was pulled through the plethysmographic chambers, a blank chamber, and a reference line connected via separate but identical air-sampling pathways. The blank chamber was used as reference to monitor ambient O2 and CO2 concentrations periodically. V̇o2 and V̇co2 values (corrected for body weight), and corresponding to 10 consecutive samples were calculated and averaged at time points representing normoxic baseline, pHVR, and HVD (see online supplement).

Arterial Blood Gases

In a subset of 15-day-old rats, PE-10 catheters were introduced into the abdominal aorta under general anesthesia (pentobarbital [Nembutal], 30 mg/kg, intraperitoneal), and exteriorized in the dorsal aspect of the neck as previously described (22). Arterial blood samples were obtained from the implanted arterial catheter in the rats. After withdrawal of 75–100 μl of blood in the dead space of the catheter, another 150 μl was sampled for immediate analysis of Po2, Pco2, and pH with a blood gas analyzer (ABL150; Radiometer, Copenhagen, Denmark).

Morris Water Maze

The Morris water maze was configured to test performance that reflects types of learning and memory traditionally defined as spatial reference, spatial working, and nonspatial reference (23, 24). A Plexiglas escape platform (12 cm in diameter) was positioned 2 cm below the water surface and placed at various locations throughout the pool. The platform was retracted manually during probe trials. A circular black curtain surrounded the pool and extended from the ceiling to the floor. For spatial tasks that depended on allocentric strategies, white extramaze cues were hung from the curtain at fixed locations. Maze performances were recorded by a video camera. For water maze experiments, only male rats were tested to prevent potential variability introduced by the estrous cycle on maze learning (25). Animals were tested at 30 days, an age at which no sex differences occur (26), and subsequently at 120 days of age (an age that clearly exhibits sex-dependent effects [25]). One day before place learning, male rats were habituated to the water maze during a free swim. Place learning was assessed on four daily training trials separated by 180 seconds over 4 days. On a daily session, each littermate was placed into the pool from four quasi-random start points and allowed a maximum of 90 seconds to escape to the platform, where he was allowed to remain for 15 seconds. Rats that failed to escape were led to the platform. The position of the platform remained constant across trials. After the final training trial of each day, the platform was retracted for a 30-second probe trial during which the time spent in each of the four quadrants of the tank and the number of target crossings over the previous location of the platform were recorded. Probe trials thus provide a measure of spatial bias developed during learning. To assess performance during place training, mean escape latencies, swim distances, and swim speeds were analyzed by two-way ANOVA (hypoxia condition, four-trial block) with repeated measures on block, followed by Newman–Keuls tests when appropriate. In addition, the occurrence of specific search strategies used during place training was noted and analyzed. To assess probe performance, mean quadrant times and target crossings were compared by two-way ANOVA (hypoxia condition and trial) with repeated measures on trial followed by Newman–Keuls tests when appropriate (see online supplement).

Eighteen pups were assessed in each experimental group for each postnatal age, and these animals were derived from a total of 19 litters.

Somatic Growth

Rat pups born to IH-exposed mothers (IHRA) weighed significantly less than control animals (RARA; Figure 1)

. However, catch-up growth occurred rapidly after birth, such that by 15 days of age, animals from both experimental groups had achieved similar weights (Figure 1).

Normoxic Ventilation

At all postnatal ages, normoxic ventilation was significantly greater in IH-exposed rat pups when compared with controls (Figure 2

; p < 0.001). This effect was long-lasting, and was still apparent at 4 months of age (IHRA versus RARA, p < 0.01). The mean values of Vt, f, V̇e, Vt/Ti, Ti/Ttot, V̇o2, V̇co2, respiratory exchange ratio, and V̇e/V̇o2 are shown in Tables 1 and 2

TABLE 1. Ventilatory and metabolic measurements in rat pups born from a normoxic pregnancy during room air, and during peak ventilatory response and hypoxic ventilatory depression in response to a 20-MINUTE hypoxic challenge



5 Days

10 Days

RA
pHVR
HVD
RA
pHVR
HVD
VT0.0057 ± 0.00030.0066 ± 0.00020.0047 ± 0.00020.0067 ± 0.00050.0077 ± 0.00030.0039 ± 0.0004
f180.9 ± 2.8247.5 ± 6.3130.8 ± 5.7205.4 ± 13.9282.8 ± 15.8170.4 ± 19.2
V̇E1.00 ± 0.03*1.52 ± 0.050.61 ± 0.02*1.34 ± 0.092.18 ± 0.150.59 ± 0.07
VT/TI0.068 ± 0.0020.088 ± 0.0020.034 ± 0.0010.074 ± 0.0060.092 ± 0.0070.034 ± 0.002
TI/Ttot0.22 ± 0.010.27 ± 0.010.20 ± 0.010.27 ± 0.020.30 ± 0.020.22 ± 0.02
VO261.4 ± 2.850.7 ± 2.436.4 ± 1.857.5 ± 2.546.4 ± 1.939.8 ± 2.0
V̇CO252.7 ± 1.845.4 ± 1.737.6 ± 1.144.5 ± 1.340.2 ± 1.442.7 ± 1.8
RER0.86 ± 0.040.89 ± 0.061.03 ± 0.060.85 ± 0.050.87 ± 0.051.07 ± 0.07
V̇E/V̇O216.2 ± 1.729.9 ± 2.416.8 ± 1.423.3 ± 2.046.9 ± 3.914.8 ± 2.7
Weight12.3 ± 0.3*21.0 ± 1.2*
15 Days
30 Days

RA
pHVR
HVD
RA
pHVR
HVD
VT0.0084 ± 0.00020.0094 ± 0.00040.0046 ± 0.00030.0093 ± 0.00050.0106 ± 0.00080.0078 ± 0.0007
f148.6 ± 19.8220.7 ± 13.5151.3 ± 10.5127.5 ± 2.5258.8 ± 19.6134.1 ± 6.2
V̇E1.25 ± 0.082.07 ± 0.170.69 ± 0.101.19 ± 0.062.75 ± 0.301.05 ± 0.11
VT/TI0.062 ± 0.0070.099 ± 0.0070.042 ± 0.0050.046 ± 0.0020.1129 ± 0.0120.048 ± 0.005
TI/Ttot0.34 ± 0.020.35 ± 0.020.28 ± 0.010.41 ± 0.010.44 ± 0.010.33 ± 0.01
VO244.8 ± 2.940.2 ± 2.437.0 ± 2.137.4 ± 1.538.4 ± 2.137.5 ± 2.0
V̇CO237.4 ± 1.938.5 ± 1.939.4 ± 2.431.0 ± 1.140.5 ± 1.941.2 ± 2.3
RER0.86 ± 0.060.95 ± 0.071.07 ± 0.070.84 ± 0.041.05 ± 0.071.10 ± 0.08
V̇E/V̇O227.9 ± 2.451.5 ± 3.118.7 ± 1.531.8 ± 2.671.6 ± 4.828.2 ± 2.3
Weight


25.8 ± 0.3


81.4 ± 2.5

* p Value < 0.01, intermittent hypoxia and room air versus normoxia.

Definition of abbreviations: f = respiratory frequency; HVD = hypoxic ventilatory depression; pHVR = peak ventilatory response to hypoxia; RA = room air; RER = respiratory exchange ratio.

n = 18/group.

TABLE 2. Ventilatory and metabolic measurements in rat pups born after an intermittent hypoxia pregnancy during room air, and during peak ventilatory response and hypoxic ventilatory depression in response to a 20-MINUTE hypoxic challenge



5 Days

10 Days

RA
pHVR
HVD
RA
pHVR
HVD
VT0.0070* ± 0.00040.0077 ± 0.00060.0038 ± 0.00030.0066 ± 0.00040.0079 ± 0.00040.0035 ± 0.0002
f 174.2 ± 6.1210.7 ± 15.1115.3 ± 14.0229.9 ± 16.8300.7 ± 18.0162.9 ± 14.2
V̇E 1.23 ± 0.08*1.62 ± 0.12*0.44 ± 0.03*1.51 ± 0.07*2.39 ± 0.180.57 ± 0.06
VT/TI 0.066 ± 0.0030.085 ± 0.0030.028 ± 0.0010.079 ± 0.0040.111 ± 0.0060.033 ± 0.004
TI/Ttot 0.24 ± 0.010.29 ± 0.020.191 ± 0.010.28 ± 0.010.33 ± 0.010.27 ± 0.03
V̇O2 60.4 ± 2.549.4 ± 2.330.1 ± 1.956.2 ± 2.847.2 ± 2.041.7 ± 2.1
V̇CO2 54.3 ± 2.149.2 ± 2.438.4 ± 1.848.7 ± 2.440.9 ± 1.644.6 ± 2.2
RER 0.87 ± 0.040.99 ± 0.071.25 ± 0.090.87 ± 0.050.88 ± 0.061.08 ± 0.08
V̇E/V̇O2 20.1 ± 1.8*32.3 ± 2.5*14.6 ± 1.2*26.9 ± 2.1*49.8 ± 3.813.8 ± 1.8
Weight 8.6 ± 0.8*14.5 ± 0.5*
15 Days
30 Days

RA
pHVR
HVD
RA
pHVR
HVD
VT0.0084 ± 0.00020.0089 ± 0.00030.0043 ± 0.00060.0117 ± 0.00100.0127 ± 0.00100.0095 ± 0.0009
f173.1 ± 12.3264.7 ± 20.0179.2 ± 15.5127.8 ± 2.9259.8 ± 20.6134.9 ± 16.5
V̇E1.46 ± 0.09*2.35 ± 0.210.78 ± 0.171.49 ± 0.13*3.29 ± 0.291.271 ± 0.15
VT/TI0.066 ± 0.0060.109 ± 0.0090.043 ± 0.0080.057 ± 0.0040.111 ± 0.0130.058 ± 0.006
TI/Ttot0.37 ± 0.010.35 ± 0.010.29 ± 0.010.41 ± 0.010.42 ± 0.010.33 ± 0.011
V̇O245.2 ± 3.043.2 ± 2.739.1 ± 2.237.6 ± 1.842.1 ± 2.140.2 ± 2.4
V̇CO237.9 ± 2.139.2 ± 2.141.2 ± 2.331.6 ± 1.440.9 ± 2.044.5 ± 2.6
RER0.83 ± 0.060.91 ± 0.081.05 ± 0.090.84 ± 0.060.98 ± 0.091.11 ± 0.10
V̇E/V̇O232.3 ± 2.3*54.6 ± 3.419.8 ± 1.939.6 ± 2.1*78.3 ± 5.331.0 ± 2.7
Weight
25.7 ± 0.8


78.4 ± 3.8


* p Value < 0.01, intermittent hypoxia and room air versus normoxia.

Definition of abbreviations: f = respiratory frequency; HVD = hypoxic ventilatory depression; pHVR = peak ventilatory response to hypoxia; RA = room air; RER = respiratory exchange ratio; TI = inspiratory duration; Ttot = duration of respiratory cycle.

n = 18/group.

for both groups at each postnatal age. Increases in ventilation were preferentially achieved through Vt increases (p < 0.01, ANOVA) at 5 days of age, but not at later ages. There were no differences in oxygen consumption between IHRA and RARA, such that the ventilatory equivalents (V̇e/V̇o2) revealed relative hyperventilation at all ages for IHRA rats (p < 0.01, ANOVA). Arterial blood gases during normoxic breathing were obtained in 15-day-old rats. Mean PaO2 was 102.7 ± 4.2 mm Hg (n = 6) in IHRA and 103.6 ± 3.9 mm Hg (n = 8) in RARA rats (p = not significant). Similarly, pH was 7.36 ± 0.03 in IHRA and 7.39 ± 0.02 in RARA (p = not significant). However, PaCO2 was 30.6 ± 1.7 mm Hg in IHRA and 34.8 ± 1.9 mm Hg in RARA (p < 0.02).

Hypoxic Ventilatory Responses
Peak HVR.

In general, the absolute values of peak minute ventilation (pHVR) were similar between IHRA- and RARA-exposed pups during the hypoxic challenge (Table 2; Figure 3)

. However, when corrected for the preceding baseline normoxic ventilation, 5-day-old pups born in IHRA displayed significantly decreased pHVR (p < 0.01, ANOVA; Figure 3). This effect was not present at any subsequent postnatal age (Figure 3). The attenuation of pHVR in IHRA was mediated by both reduced respiratory frequency and tidal volume increases (p < 0.01, ANOVA). Although there were age-dependent changes in metabolic measures, there were no significant differences in V̇o2 and V̇co2 responses in the two groups during periods corresponding to pHVR. However, the V̇e/V̇o2 revealed significantly reduced respiratory recruitments in the IH gestationally exposed rats during pHVR at all ages (p < 0.05, ANOVA; Figure 4) . These findings suggest that peripheral chemoreceptor sensitivity is attenuated even at 30 days postnatally after gestational exposure to intermittent hypoxia.

HVD.

Late hypoxic ventilatory depression (HVD) was enhanced in IHRA-exposed pups at 5 days of age (Figure 3; p < 0.001), but not thereafter, and was mediated via reductions in both respiratory frequency and tidal volume. However, whereas absolute V̇o2 values were similar in the two groups during HVD, V̇e/V̇o2 relationships displayed not only significant postnatal age-related changes, but were also significantly lower in IHRA rats compared with RARA rats (p < 0.01; Figure 4). Thus, all pups exhibited a nadir of ventilatory drive during HVD at 10 days postnatally, and such ventilatory depression was particularly prominent after exposure to intermittent hypoxia during fetal life.

Dejours Test

In 5-day-old pups, 100% oxygen resulted in a mean V̇e reduction of 23.2 ± 4.7% in RARA pups and of 7.2 ± 3.5% in IHRA pups (p < 0.03). Thus, prenatal exposures to intermittent hypoxia attenuate peripheral chemoreceptor sensitivity.

Morris Water Maze

Twelve IH-exposed and 12 control male rats were studied at 30 and 120 days postnatally. No differences emerged in the ability to acquire the spatial task in the two treatment groups, such that the mean latencies and distances that the rats swam in the pool were similar across trials (Figure 5)

. Furthermore, there were no differences in the relative proximity of the rats to the putative platform position during probe trials. Thus, prenatal IH does not affect water maze performance during acquisition and retention of a spatial task.

In this study we have shown that gestational IH leads to significant reductions in fetal growth that are readily reversible within the first few days of normoxic postnatal life. The major ventilatory effect of prenatal IH consists of the sustained (more than 120 days) enhancement of normoxic ventilation that is preferentially mediated by Vt increases. Furthermore, altered HVR is readily apparent at 5 days of age, manifesting both as reduced peak ventilation during application of a moderate short-lasting poikilocapnic hypoxic stimulus and by enhanced late hypoxic ventilatory depression. However, disruption of metabolic and respiratory relationships during such acute hypoxic challenges persists until 30 days of age, and suggests long-lasting alteration of integrative adaptations to hypoxia. Finally, IH was devoid of any apparent effect on Morris water maze performance during acquisition and retention of a spatial task.

Somatic Growth

The effects of hypoxia on somatic growth have been relatively well studied. Most studies support the concept that low birthweight is a frequent result of stunted fetal growth due to placental hypoxia elicited by a sustained hypoxic environment such as in high altitude (2729). In animal models, gestational exposure to hypoxia elicited stunted fetal growth (30, 31) and, in fact, even short (1–2 hours) exposures during the last third of pregnancy in rats will result in significant reductions in fetal body weight and selectively affected organ growth (32). However, it is possible that catecholamines released via the stress response to IH, rather than hypoxia per se, may underlie the reduced fetal growth associated with gestational IH (33).

Additional evidence concerning human pregnancies suggests fetal growth retardation occurs in offspring of snoring women (11, 34). Snoring was postulated to impact on maternal cardiorespiratory reserve and indirectly on the fetus, and recordable changes in fetal heart rate and in the acid–base status of the fetus were identified (35, 36). It is possible that the altered placental function could ensue as a result of vascular changes induced by obstructive sleep apnea and the accompanying intermittent hypoxia, the latter possibly inducing or aggravating preexisting preeclampsia (10, 37). In support of this possibility, Edwards and colleagues have shown the favorable effect of continuous positive airway pressure on systemic blood pressure in pregnant women with preeclampsia (38).

The application of intermittent hypoxia during daylight, the rest–sleep period component of the circadian phase in our rat model, obviously does not completely mimic all of the alterations associated with sleep-disordered breathing, because neither hypercapnia nor sleep fragmentation is present (12). However, IH has been clearly identified as the major contributor to vascular dysfunction (39) and hypertension in sleep apnea (37, 40). Thus, it is possible that the fetal effects of intermittent hypoxia during sleep will be further exacerbated when underlying vascular dysfunction, for example, preeclampsia or diabetes mellitus, is already present.

Ventilatory Responses

Normoxic ventilation was increased after fetal intermittent hypoxia, and this effect persisted well into adulthood. However, attenuation of the ventilatory equivalent responses to acute hypoxia was also present during the first postnatal month. These long-lasting changes in respiratory control are not only indicative of the induction of respiratory plasticity, but also suggest that the latter can occur through application of conditioning stimuli in utero. Indeed, small for gestational age newborn infants who developed intrauterine growth retardation exhibit subtle, albeit significant differences in respiratory rhythm during sleep, and such differences have been attributed to alterations in brainstem development (41). Furthermore, chronic gestational hypoxia is associated with increased methionine-enkephalin and altered substance P concentrations in the pontomedullary brainstem regions (42, 43). The functional ventilatory outcome of gestational hypoxia, as evidenced from a study by Gleed and Mortola and the present study, suggests the emergence of relative hypoplasticity of lung tissue and increased basal brainstem respiratory rhythm activity, resulting in increased ventilatory output even when adjusted for underlying metabolism (44).

The mechanisms underlying the neural plasticity associated with manipulations of oxygen concentration in breathed air have not been extensively investigated, and in fact little is currently known about the effect of prenatal hypoxia on respiratory control. Before we address this particular issue however, a methodological problem pertaining to the analysis of ventilatory responses to hypoxia needs to be examined. In the context of increased normoxic ventilation, actual ventilatory measurements may be similar at pHVR, for example, yet the relative change will be smaller (e.g., 5-day-old pups; Figures 2 and 3). These ventilatory responses could be then construed as unaffected (based on actual ventilatory measures) or attenuated (based on relative change from normoxic baseline). This problem is not apparent during HVD, whereby both absolute and relative ventilation in 5-day-old pups was decreased compared with control animals (Figure 3). The excitatory respiratory plasticity that occurs after intermittent hypoxic stimulation and manifests as increased normoxic ventilation has been ascribed to the sustained induction of serotoninergic neurotransmission systems with opposing effects from downregulation of noradrenergic-dependent neural transmission (45, 46). This is in contrast with the absence of such long-term facilitation when the stimulus is continuous hypoxia (45, 47). It has been further postulated that these changes in serotonin and noradrenergic transmission will attenuate fluctuations in ventilatory drive, especially when stimuli change rapidly (48). As such, application of episodic hypoxia to the adult rodent for 7 days resulted in increased overall phrenic nerve output responses to hypoxia and to carotid sinus nerve stimulation that were dependent on serotonin receptors (49). However, episodic hypoxia may not always induce enhancements of subsequent acute hypoxic ventilatory responses. For example, exposure of 2- to 3-day-old rat pups to a series of eight cycles of IH consisting of 5 minutes of hypoxia (iO2, 0.10) and 10 minutes of normoxia revealed that the late phase of HVR but not the peak HVR was modified by the intermittent hypoxia paradigm when compared with normoxia (50). The major effects consisted of reductions in HVD that were attributed at least in part to an increased expression of the neuronal isoform of nitric oxide synthase within the caudal brainstem (50). Similarly, Trippenbach reported that repeated and successive hypoxic runs will elicit enhancements of the HVR in rabbit pups during a subsequent hypoxic challenge (51). These findings concur with the postulated increase in peripheral chemoreceptor sensitivity attributed to episodic hypoxia exposures (52). In contrast with such findings, however, Waters and Tinworth showed that in piglets repeated daily exposure to hypoxia (30 minutes/day) induced attenuated respiratory responses to a subsequent acute hypoxic stimulus (53). Our present results as derived from both hypoxic and hyperoxic exposures would suggest that peripheral chemosensitivity to hypoxia is reduced and not enhanced after long-lasting cyclical hypoxic exposures. Furthermore, such repeated hypoxic exposures also increased the interstitial concentrations of substance P and methionine-enkephalin within the nucleus of the solitary tract (42, 43, 54, 55), suggesting that in addition to serotoninergic and adrenergic systems, other respiratory-relevant neurotransmitter systems may also be affected. Because N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) glutamatergic transmission is critical to HVR in the adult rat (56), the substantial developmental changes in expression and function of the NMDA glutamate receptor that occur within critical brainstem respiratory sites (57) are responsible for maturation and plasticity of neuronal systems in multiple other brain regions (58, 59). Thus, it is possible that altered NMDA receptor transmission is elicited by IH and accounts for the findings reported herein. Indeed, in young piglets, NMDA receptor-mediated Ca2+ influx and receptor nitration will increase with hypoxic severity, and such changes in NMDA receptor function are manifest within 1 hour of a single exposure to hypoxia (60). Exposure to intermittent hypercapnic hypoxia over a period of 4 days was associated with differential changes in the expression of the NR1 NMDA receptor mRNA (increased) and NR1 protein (decreased) (61). Furthermore, in the adult rat dorsocaudal brainstem, long-lasting intermittent hypoxia (30 days) was associated with significant changes in NMDA NR2 receptor subtype expression (62). Taken together, these findings suggest that intermittent hypoxia will induce substantial alterations in multiple downstream neurotransmitters or signaling molecules (e.g., nitric oxide, protein kinase C) that modulate respiratory control, and that such changes exhibit significant dependency on the developmental stage at which they occur (63).

This study revealed that gestational IH was associated with significant attenuations of the peak hypoxic ventilatory response and enhanced late hypoxic depression in the younger neonatal rats. However, when ventilation was adjusted for oxygen consumption, IH-exposed animals displayed lower ventilatory recruitments during both phases of the hypoxic response. Interestingly, both control and IH-exposed pups displayed clear developmentally regulated patterns of ventilatory equivalent responses to hypoxia, in which late decreases in V̇e/V̇o2 were particularly prominent during postnatal ages 10–15 days. However, such decreases were enhanced after gestational IH, suggesting the possibility of an age- and IH-dependent selective vulnerable period. Indeed, early exposure to intermittent hypoxia will lead to significant alterations in the gasping responses to asphyxia and in the ability to autoresuscitate (64). Qualitatively similar results were reported in the newborn piglet metabolic response to intermittent cycles of hypoxia, whereby age-dependent failure of ventilatory and metabolic adaptations emerged at 2 weeks of age and manifested as more severe lactic acidosis (65), and in rats exposed prenatally to sustained hypoxia (2). Thus, further exploration of the conceptual framework of fetal IH-induced postnatal selective hypoxic vulnerability may help to explain why face-down positioning and repetitive apnea appear to lead to sudden infant death syndrome in only selected infants.

Water Maze

The absence of any alterations in water maze performance in IH-exposed rats was somewhat unexpected despite the relatively mild magnitude of the hypoxic stimulus. Indeed, significant differences in neurobehavioral performance have been previously noted following hypoxia during gestation (3, 5). Most of these differences, however, manifest as subtle alterations in gait behaviors and sexual maturation, particularly testosterone regulation (4, 5), rather than as detectable changes in water maze task acquisition or retention. In addition, Nyakas and colleagues have also shown that abnormal open-field, social, learning, and emotional behaviors, as well as altered plasma corticosterone responses to stress, occur throughout adulthood after exposure to prenatal hypoxia (66, 67). Similarly, when the IH paradigm used in the present study was applied to 10-day-old postnatal rat pups for 15 days, marked decreases in the ability to acquire and retain a spatial task in the water maze emerged (26). Thus, either the gestational IH profile does not lead to detectable neurobehavioral deficits in the offspring, or these changes, if indeed present, are readily reversed during the first month of life (i.e., representing a form of compensatory neural plasticity), and do not impose long-lasting changes in the acquisition and retention of spatial function.

In summary, we have shown that the occurrence of gestational intermittent hypoxia is associated with prolonged and a priori irreversible alterations in respiratory control, but does not induce any detectable obvious neurocognitive deficits. We postulate that intermittent hypoxia during pregnancy may lead to maladaptive responses to postnatal hypoxia and potentially increase the vulnerability to fatal conditions such as sudden infant death syndrome in selected infants.

1. Bavis RW, Kilgore DL Jr. Effects of embryonic CO2 exposure on the adult ventilatory response in quail: does gender matter? Respir Physiol 2001;126:183–199.
2. Peyronnet J, Roux JC, Geloen A, Tang LQ, Pequignot JM, Lagercrantz H, Dalmaz Y. Prenatal hypoxia impairs the postnatal development of neural and functional chemoafferent pathway in rat. J Physiol 2000;524:525–537.
3. Hermans RH, Hunter DE, McGivern RF, Cain CD, Longo LD. Behavioral sequelae in young rats of acute intermittent antenatal hypoxia. Neurotoxicol Teratol 1992;14:119–129.
4. Hermans RH, Longo LD, McGivern RF. Decreased postnatal testosterone and corticosterone concentrations in rats following acute intermittent prenatal hypoxia without alterations in adult male sex behavior. Neurotoxicol Teratol 1994;16:201–206.
5. Hermans RH, McGivern RF, Chen W, Longo LD. Altered adult sexual behavior in the male rat following chronic prenatal hypoxia. Neurotoxicol Teratol 1993;15:353–363.
6. Levitt P, Reinoso B, Jones L. The critical impact of early cellular environment on neuronal development. Prev Med 1998;27:180–183.
7. Fuller DD, Bavis RW, Vidruk EH, Wang ZY, Olson EB Jr, Bisgard GE, Mitchell GS. Life-long impairment of hypoxic phrenic responses in rats following 1 month of developmental hyperoxia. J Physiol 2002;538:947–955.
8. Okubo S, Mortola JP. Control of ventilation in adult rats hypoxic in the neonatal period. Am J Physiol 1990;259:R836–R841.
9. Sladek M, Parker RA, Groogard JB, Sundell HW. Long-lasting effect of prolonged hypoxemia after birth on the immediate ventilatory response to changes in arterial partial pressure of oxygen in young lambs. Pediatr Res 1993;34:821–828.
10. Edwards N, Blyton DM, Kirjavainen TT, Sullivan CE. Hemodynamic responses to obstructive respiratory events during sleep are augmented in women with preeclampsia. Am J Hypertens 2001;14:1090–1095.
11. Loube DI, Poceta JS, Morales MC, Peacock MD, Mitler MM. Self-reported snoring in pregnancy: association with fetal outcome. Chest 1996;109:885–889.
12. Gozal D, Daniel JM, Dohanich GP. Behavioral and anatomical correlates of chronic episodic hypoxia during sleep in the rat. J Neurosci 2001;21:2442–2450.
13. Gozal E, Row BW, Schurr A, Gozal D. Developmental differences in cortical and hippocampal vulnerability to intermittent hypoxia in the rat. Neurosci Lett 2001;305:197–201.
14. National Institutes of Health. Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. Bethesda, MD: 1985. DHHS, PHS, NIH Publication No. 85-23.
15. Bartlett DJB, Tenney SW. Control of breathing in experimental anemia. Respir Physiol 1970;10:384–395.
16. Pappenheimer J. Sleep and respiration of rats during hypoxia. J Physiol 1977;266:191–207.
17. Epstein RA, Epstein MAF, Haddad GG, Mellins RB. Practical implementation of the barometric method for measurement of tidal volume. J Appl Physiol 1980;49:1107–1115.
18. Bandla HPR, Simakajornboon N, Graff GR, Gozal D. Protein kinase C modulates ventilatory patterning in the developing rat. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 1999;159:968–973.
19. Drorbaugh JE, Fenn WO. A barometric method for measuring ventilation in newborn infants. Pediatrics 1955;16:81–87.
20. Bamford OS, Carroll JL. Dynamic ventilatory responses in rats: normal development and effects of prenatal nicotine exposure. Respir Physiol 1999;117:29–40.
21. Jensen DR, Gayles EC, Ammon S, Phillips R, Eckel RH. A self-correcting indirect calorimeter system for the measurement of energy balance in small animals. J Appl Physiol 2001;90:912–918.
22. Gozal E, Roussel AL, Holt GA, Gozal L, Gozal YM, Torres JE, Gozal D. Protein kinase C modulation of the ventilatory response to hypoxia in the nucleus tractus solitarius of the conscious rat. J Appl Physiol 1998;84:1982–1990.
23. Morris RGM. Spatial localization does not require the presence of local cues. Learn Motiv 1981;12:239–260.
24. Morris RGM. Development of a water-maze procedure for studying spatial learning in the rat. J Neurosci Methods 1984;11:47–60.
25. Flood JF, Morley JE, Roberts E. Memory-enhancing effects in male mice of pregnenolone and steroids metabolically derived from it. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1992;89:1567–1571.
26. Row BW, Kheirandish L, Neville JJ, Gozal D. Impaired spatial learning and hyperactivity in developing rats exposed to intermittent hypoxia. Pediatr Res 2002;52:449–453.
27. Khalid ME, Ali ME, Ali KZ. Full-term birth weight and placental morphology at high and low altitude. Int J Gynaecol Obstet 1997;57:259–265.
28. Giussani DA, Phillips PS, Anstee S, Barker DJ. Effects of altitude versus economic status on birth weight and body shape at birth. Pediatr Res 2001;49:490–494.
29. McCullough RE, Reeves JT. Fetal growth retardation and increased infant mortality at high altitude. Arch Environ Health 1977;32:36–39.
30. Chang JH, Rutledge JC, Stoops D, Abbe R. Hypobaric hypoxia-induced intrauterine growth retardation. Biol Neonate 1984;46:10–13.
31. de Grauw TJ, Myers RE, Scott WJ. Fetal growth retardation in rats from different levels of hypoxia. Biol Neonate 1986;49:85–89.
32. Schwartz JE, Kovach A, Meyer J, McConnell C, Iwamoto HS. Brief, intermittent hypoxia restricts fetal growth in Sprague-Dawley rats. Biol Neonate 1998;73:313–319.
33. Bassett JM, Hanson MC. Catecholamines inhibit growth in fetal sheep in the absence of hypoxemia. Am J Physiol 1998;274:R1536–R1545.
34. Franklin KA, Holmgren PA, Jonsson F, Poromaa N, Stenlund H, Svanborg E. Snoring, pregnancy-induced hypertension, and growth retardation of the fetus. Chest 2000;117:137–141.
35. Joel-Cohen SJ, Schoenfeld A. Fetal response to periodic sleep apnea: a new syndrome in obstetrics. Eur J Obstet Gynecol Reprod Biol 1978;8:77–81.
36. Schoenfeld A, Ovadia Y, Neri A, Freedman S. Obstructive sleep apnea (OSA): implications in maternal–fetal medicine. A hypothesis. Med Hypotheses 1989;30:51–54.
37. Fletcher EC. Physiological consequences of intermittent hypoxia: systemic blood pressure [invited review]. J Appl Physiol 2001;90:1600–1605.
38. Edwards N, Blyton DM, Kirjavainen T, Kesby GJ, Sullivan CE. Nasal continuous positive airway pressure reduces sleep-induced blood pressure increments in preeclampsia. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 2000;162:252–257.
39. Tahawi Z, Orolinova N, Joshua IG, Bader M, Fletcher EC. Altered vascular reactivity in arterioles of chronic intermittent hypoxic rats. J Appl Physiol 2001;90:2007–2013.
40. Bakehe M, Hedner J, Dang T, Chambille B, Gaultier C, Escourrou P. Role of the autonomic nervous system in the acute blood pressure elevation during repetitive hypoxic and hypercapnic breathing in rats. Blood Press 1996;5:371–375.
41. Curzi-Dascalova L, Peirano P, Christova E. Respiratory characteristics during sleep in healthy small-for- gestational age newborns. Pediatrics 1996;97:554–559.
42. Gingras JL, Long WA. Chronic maternal hypoxia: effect of mid-gestational maternal hypoxia on methionine-enkephalin concentrations within pre- and postnatal rabbit brainstem regions. Dev Neurosci 1988;10:180–189.
43. Gingras JL, Long WA, Segreti T, Wasserstein M. Pre- and postnatal effects of chronic maternal hypoxia on substance-P immunoreactivity in rabbit brainstem regions. Dev Neurosci 1995;17:350–356.
44. Gleed RD, Mortola JP. Ventilation in newborn rats after gestation at simulated high altitude. J Appl Physiol 1991;70:1146–1151.
45. Baker TL, Fuller DD, Zabka AG, Mitchell GS. Respiratory plasticity: differential actions of continuous and episodic hypoxia and hypercapnia. Respir Physiol 2001;129:25–35.
46. Baker-Herman TL, Mitchel GS. Phrenic long-term facilitation requires spinal serotonin receptor activation and protein synthesis. J Neurosci 2002;22:6239–6246.
47. Baker TL, Mitchell GS. Episodic but not continuous hypoxia elicits long-term facilitation of phrenic motor output in rats. J Physiol 2000;529:215–219.
48. Kinkead R, Bach KB, Johnson SM, Hodgeman BA, Mitchell GS. Plasticity in respiratory motor control: intermittent hypoxia and hypercapnia activate opposing serotonergic and noradrenergic modulatory systems. Comp Biochem Physiol A Mol Integr Physiol 2001;130:207–218.
49. Ling L, Fuller DD, Bach KB, Kinkead R, Olson EB Jr, Mitchell GS. Chronic intermittent hypoxia elicits serotonin-dependent plasticity in the central neural control of breathing. J Neurosci 2001;21:5381–5388.
50. Gozal D, Gozal E. Episodic hypoxia potentiates the late hypoxic ventilatory response in the developing rat: putative role of neuronal nitric oxide synthase. Am J Physiol 1999;276:R17–R22.
51. Trippenbach T. Ventilatory and metabolic effects of repeated hypoxia in conscious newborn rabbits. Am J Physiol 1994;266:R1584–R590.
52. Prabhakar NR. Oxygen sensing during intermittent hypoxia: cellular and molecular mechanisms. J Appl Physiol 2001;90:1986–1994.
53. Waters KA, Tinworth KD. Depression of ventilatory responses after daily, cyclic hypercapnic hypoxia in piglets. J Appl Physiol 2001;90:1065–1073.
54. Rodier ME, Laferriere A, Moss IR. Effects of age and clustered hypoxia on [125I]substance P binding to neurotachykinin-1 receptors in brainstem of developing swine. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 2001;127:31–39.
55. Yan S, Zhang C, Laferriere A, Moss IR. Met-enkephalin-like immunoreactivity in microdialysates from nucleus tractus solitarii in piglets during normoxia and hypoxia. Brain Res 1995;687:217–220.
56. Ohtake PJ, Torres JE, Gozal YM, Graff GR, Gozal D. NMDA receptors mediate cardiorespiratory responses to afferent peripheral chemoreceptor input in the conscious rat. J Appl Physiol 1998;84:853–861.
57. Ohtake PJ, Simakajornboon N, Fehniger MD, Xue YD, Gozal D. NMDA receptor expression in the nucleus tractus solitarii and maturation of hypoxic ventilatory response in the rat. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 2000;162:1140–1147.
58. Herlenius E, Lagercrantz H. Neurotransmitters and neuromodulators during early human development. Early Hum Dev 2001;65:21–37.
59. Panigrahy A, Rosenberg PA, Assmann S, Foley EC, Kinney HC. Differential expression of glutamate receptor subtypes in human brainstem sites involved in perinatal hypoxia–ischemia. J Comp Neurol 2000;427:196–208.
60. Zanelli SA, Ashraf QM, Mishra OP. Nitration is a mechanism of regulation of the NMDA receptor function during hypoxia. Neuroscience 2002;112:869–877.
61. Machaalani R, Waters KA. Distribution and quantification of NMDA R1 mRNA and protein in the piglet brainstem and effects of intermittent hypercapnic hypoxia (IHH). Brain Res 2002;951:293–300.
62. Holbrook M, Reeves SR, Wang M, Lipton AJ, Sachleben LR Jr, Gozal E, Gozal D. Differential effects of sustained and intermittent hypoxia on rostrocaudal brainstem NMDA glutamate receptor subunit expression: implications for acute hypoxic ventilatory response [abstract]. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 2002;165:A668.
63. Gozal E, Gozal D. Respiratory plasticity following intermittent hypoxia: developmental interactions. J Appl Physiol 2001;90:1995–1999.
64. Gozal D, Gozal E, Reeves SR, Lipton AJ. Gasping and autoresuscitation in the developing rat: effect of antecedent intermittent hypoxia. J Appl Physiol 2002;92:1141–1144.
65. Cote A, Barter J, Meehan B. Age-dependent metabolic effects of repeated hypoxemia in piglets. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 2000;78:321–328.
66. Nyakas C, Buwalda B, Kramers RJ, Traber J, Luiten PG. Postnatal development of hippocampal and neocortical cholinergic and serotonergic innervation in rat: effects of nitrite-induced prenatal hypoxia and nimodipine treatment. Neuroscience 1994;59:541–559.
67. Nyakas C, Buwalda B, Markel E, Korte SM, Luiten PG. Life-spanning behavioural and adrenal dysfunction induced by prenatal hypoxia in the rat is prevented by the calcium antagonist nimodipine. Eur J Neurosci 1994;6:746–753.
Correspondence and requests for reprints should be addressed to Dr. David Gozal, M.D., Kosair Children's Hospital Research Institute, University of Louisville, 570 S. Preston Street, Ste. 321, Louisville, KY 40202. E-mail:

Related

No related items
American Journal of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine
167
11

Click to see any corrections or updates and to confirm this is the authentic version of record